top of page
Active Projects
Previous Projects
Forthcoming Projects

ACTIVE PROJECTS

Traumatic brain injury

Effects of sports-related concussion on the neurocognitive-linguistic system

The short- and long-term effects of sports-related concussions remain a growing concern. Although the majority of college athletes' symptoms and cognitive deficits typically recover within 1-2 weeks post-concussion, findings from our lab (and others) used ERPs to demonstrate persistent changes to the functional neural networks underlying attention (Ledwidge & Molfese, 2016) and working memory (Hudac, Cortesa, Ledwidge & Molfese, 2017) years following injury. However, a lack of difference in neuropsychological abilities between athletes with and without a history of concussion begs the question: What is the clinical utility of ERPs if behavioral markers of cognition are not affected?

Recently, we suggested that investigations studying the association between sports-related concussion and language abilities are warranted (Ledwidge, in press).  Sparse research has considered this avenue, which is surprising given the number of studies demonstrating persistent language deficits following non-sports-related concussion. Efficient and accurate language comprehension involves the interaction between both lexical, cognitive, (and often social)  mechanisms. Consequently, it is possible that behavioral and electrophysiological assessments of language comprehension provide naturalistic tools to examine recovery from sports-related concussion

We are collaborating with the BW Department of Communication Disorders to study changes to the cognitive-linguistic system during concussion recovery in college athletes. We are collecting both functional measures as well as recording ERPs during both comprehension and cognitive inhibition. Our goal is to successfully chart the time course of recovery of cognition and language in this population to better inform clinical best practices for concussion management and intervention.

Researchers adjusting an EEG net

Cognitive-Emotional  Development

Cognitive and Emotional Brain Responses in Children and Adolescents 

In this study we’re recording event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in children and adolescents 7-17 years of age to examine typically developing cognitive and emotional processing. The findings from this study will guide our future work in investigating how these brain responses may change after pediatric concussion. As shown in the picture

to the right, we

record EEG while children/adolescents view

pictures that vary in emotional

valence (i.e., negative, neutral, positive).


Before we could study the LPP brain response, we first

normed a set of images on emotional valence and arousal

children/adolescents and were also “parent-approved.”

The images we selected are from the Nencki

Affective Picture System (NAPS), which is a set

of standardized, realistic images

(e.g., animals; see right image ), that elicit

emotional responses (Marchewka et al., 2013).

These images had never been normed in

children/adolescents. In an online study, parents

determined which pictures were appropriate for 

their children to view. Then the children rated each picture

on emotional valence (e.g., negative,neutral, positive) using

the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM; Bradley & Lang, 1994; see image below). Through this study, we normed standardized a set of 60 images: 30 which elicited positive emotions in children/adolescents and 30 which were neutral (Ledwidge et al., unpublished). 

 

Previous literature has shown that mild traumatic brain injuries (mTBIs) may affect ERP markers of emotional-cognition in adults (Ma ̈ki-Marttunen et al., 2015). However, this has yet to be demonstrated in pediatric mTBI/concussion. In our study, we seek to establish that our set of parent-approved pictures replicates previous literature which showed that children’s brains elicit the LPP to emotional stimuli (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Hajcak and Olvet, 2008; Hajcak & Dennis, 2009). Our future work aims to use the LPP as a marker to study cognitive-emotional changes after concussion in children/adolescents. 

IMG_7694.jpg
Animals_175_h.jpg

Example NAPs image

Picture1.png

Example of SAM scale used to assess valence

Adult Cognition and Language

Contextual ambiguity resolution during discourse comprehension

Our understanding of the meaning being portrayed within a conversation or discourse changes to each instance of a new semantic item. However, when the topic of a conversation is ambiguous, we must actively search for and identify the meaning/purpose that interlocultors or narrators are attempting to portray. This ambiguity extends beyond single words but rather encompasses the broader discourse context. We are using ERPs to study how the brain resolves this contextual ambiguity. Furthermore, is this process distinct from that which involves the change/update of an existing context?

Preliminary results from this study suggest that the Late Anterior Positivity ("Frontal post-N400 positivity") fluctuates to coherent words that partially resolve contextual ambiguity. In contrast, greater P600 amplitudes are recorded to coherent, but unexpected words within a known, existing context.

Attention

Smartphone-induced divided attention

The literature is rich in its demonstrations of the distracting properties of your cell-phone when used while driving. It turns out that humans are not as good at multi-tasking as we think. In fact, the term "multi-tasking" is a misnomer, as we instead switch between tasks with the performance on each task decreasing as a function of the number of tasks being performed.

The social aspects of smartphones (e.g., texting, notifications) have the capacity to provide us with potentially limitless positive reinforcement. The Motivated Cognition Model (Lang, 2006) demonstrates that we allocate greater attentional resources to stimuli we are motivated to engage in, such as those which provide positive reinforcement (e.g., cell phones). And as long our phones are turned on, there is always the opportunity to receive this gratification. This study is the first to examine if and how the mere presence of a smartphones alters ERP markers of attention.

Attention

The competition between emotional and spatial attention

Through this project we seek to understand the attentional competition between emotional and spatial information. From Posner's (1980) work (And others) we know that targets presented in locations (e.g., left side of screen) that were correctly cued by a preceding arrow (pointed left) are processes quicker than when there was a mismatch between cue and target. Follow up work from Mangun and Hillyard (1991) demonstrated this attentional facilitation by recording a greater N100 ERP to correctly cued targets.

We've adapted the Posner (1980) cueing task and instead are using averted faces (eye gaze left, right) crossed with emotional expression (negative, positive, neutral)  as cues for the location of a forthcoming target. Since these cues carry both emotional and spatial information, ERPs recorded to post-cue targets will allow us to establish the attentional competition between them. The image below is an example of a valid cue-target trial.

We suspect that when an emotional face (e.g., happy, sad) validly cues the target location (e.g., eye gaze to left side, target location presented on the left), we will observe a smaller N1 amplitude to the target compared to when the valid face cue had a neutral expression. In ERP-ing the facial cue as well,dDifferences in late positive potential (LPP) amplitude to emotional faces (relative to neutral) would empirically validate that the diminished N100 effect ( to targets) is in direct consequence to the emotional information.

Untitled.png

Previous Projects

A History of mTBI in College-aged Contact Sport Athletes and The Default Mode Network

Previous research has identified that a history of diagnosed mTBI is associated with alterations to different brain networks (Arciniega et al., 2021, Hristopulos et al., 2019, Tao et al, 2015, Wang et al., 2017).  One network in particular is the default mode network, which is a resting state network.  This means that it is active or “on” in mind wandering, self-referential processing, daydreaming, etc. (Raichle, 2011). The default mode network is “off” when engaged in active tasks (Raichle, 2011).  It remains to be investigated how the default mode network is influenced by a history of mTBI in college athletes who participate in contact sports, including football, rugby, soccer, lacrosse, basketball, baseball, softball, and wrestling (Meehan et al., 2015). 

 

The focus of this project is to investigate how alpha and theta brain frequencies are altered in the default mode network and whether these alterations may be influenced by a history of diagnosed mTBI.  Alpha and theta frequencies have been cited in previous literature as being hallmarks of baseline default mode network function in resting state electroencephalography (RS-EEG) research (Bowman et al., 2017, Laufs et al., 2003, Prestel et al., 2018, Sheeringa et al., 2007).  Therefore, we will be recording 10 minutes of eyes open RS-EEG (participants will fixate on a cross) in order to look indirectly at the default mode network through the aforementioned frequency bands.

bottom of page